| | |
|
Gregory M. Herek is a Professor of Psychology at the University of California at Davis (UCD). He received his Ph.D. in social psychology from UCD in 1983, then was a postdoctoral fellow at Yale University. He co-edited Hate Crimes: Confronting Violence Against Lesbians and Gay Men (1992), and edited Stigma and Sexual Orientation (1998).
| |
Social scientists attempting to explain why so many people hold negative
feelings toward homosexual persons have tended to offer either theoretical
speculations or empirical data, with little synthesis of the two. The
theoretical accounts often have revealed more about the writer's personal
prejudices toward homosexuality than society's reaction to it. For example,
William James (1890) assumed that being repulsed by the idea of intimate
contact with a member of the same sex is instinctive, and exists more strongly
in men than in women, Interestingly, in cultures where such forms of
''unnatural vice" as homosexuality are found, James supposed that the
instinctual aversion had been overcome by habit. In other words, he assumed
that tolerance is learned and revulsion is inborn. rather than vice-versa. This
is particularly surprising in view of his hypothesis that a ''germinal
possibility'' for same-sex attraction exists in ''most men."
There are similarities between the thinking of James and Edward Westermarck
(1908), but the latter went beyond instinct-based explanations in his
cross-cultural study of morality. He was willing to assert that societal
censure of homosexual practices is due to "the feeling of aversion or
disgust which the idea of homosexual intercourse tends to call forth in
normally constituted adult individuals whose sexual instincts have developed
under normal conditions." But he thought this explanation was inadequate in
accounting for the particularly violent reaction against homosexuality
displayed by the Jewish, Christian, and Zoroastrian religions. Their strong
hostility exists, he said, because homosexual practices were associated
historically with idolatry and heresy, and so were condemned by way of laws and
customs.
Psychoanalysts offered a more social-psychological view. Sigmund Freud (1905)
asserted that an exclusive heterosexual orientation does not only result from
biological causes, but also is influenced by societal prohibitions on
homoerotism and by early experiences with parents. He assumed that all men and
women had strong attractions to their same-sex parent but these feelings were
usually repressed in dissolving the complete Oedipus complex. In many cases,
however, the repression is incomplete. Thus, Sandor Ferenczi (1914) suggested
that heterosexual men's feelings of aversion, hostility, and disgust toward
male homosexuality really are reaction-formations and symptomatic of defense
against affection for the same sex. Ferenczi did not extend his analysis to
women's attitudes or to attitudes toward lesbians, but similar processes might
be inferred.
The bulk of studies have sought to uncover the correlates of negative
attitudes. Some findings are contradictory, such as the relationship between
sex-role conformity (i.e., masculinity, femininity, androgyny) and attitudes.
In general, however, some consistent patterns have been observed across
different samples. When compared to those with more favorable attitudes toward
lesbians and gay men, these studies have found that persons with negative
attitudes:
1. are less likely to have had personal contact with lesbians or gay;
2. are less likely to report having engaged in homosexual behaviors, or to
identify themselves as lesbian or gay;
3. are more likely to perceive their peers as manifesting negative attitudes,
especially if the respondents are males;
4. are more likely to have resided in areas where negative attitudes are the
norm (e.g., the midwestern and southern United States, the Canadian prairies,
and in rural areas or small towns), especially during adolescence;
5. are likely to be older and less well educated;
6. are more likely to be religious, to attend church frequently, and to
subscribe to a conservative religious ideology;
7. are more likely to express traditional, restrictive attitudes about sex
roles;
8. are less permissive sexually or manifest more guilt or negativity about
sexuality, although some researchers have not observed this pattern and others
have reported a substantially reduced correlation with the effects of sex-role
attitudes partialled out;
9. are more likely to manifest high levels of authoritarianism and related
personality characteristics.
Sex differences in the direction and intensity of attitudes have been observed
fairly consistently. It appears that heterosexuals tend to have more negative
attitudes toward homosexuals of their own sex than of the opposite sex. . .
.
Experiential attitudes develop when affects and cognitions associated with
specific interpersonal interactions are generalized to all lesbians and
gay men. A person with positive experiences, therefore, expresses generally
favorable attitudes and a person with negative experiences reports unfavorable
favorable attitudes because of the experiences. Note that experiential
attitudes do not inevitably follow interactions. It is necessary also that
those interactions themselves (rather than, for example, ideological
considerations )
provide the primary basis for the attitude. Interactions have consequences for
both beliefs and affects associated with lesbians and gay men. Because they
provide information, face-to face interactions tend to refute stereotypes and
reduce ignorance, which Marmor (1980) identified as the most important sources
of hostility toward homosexual persons. At the same time, interpersonal
encounters have an emotional impact that individuals can generalize to all
lesbians and gay men. Thus, heterosexuals who know lesbians and gay men are
better able than others to recognize stereotypes as inaccurate, and are more
likely to express tolerant attitudes as well. Since only about one-fourth of
the adults in the United States report that they have homosexual friends or
acquaintances (Newsweek Poll, 1983), it can be hypothesized that attitudes will
become more favorable overall as more lesbians and gay men disclose their
sexual orientation to friends or family. For the present, however, we must
assume that only a minority of people in the United States have attitudes based
on experience. The remainder have formed their opinions and beliefs without the
benefit of personal contact. Consequently, stereotypical beliefs about gay men
and lesbians are prevalent, and it is appropriate here to discuss their forms and effects.
Most common stereotypes are related to cross-sex characteristics.
Additionally, significant numbers of individuals characterize male homosexuals
as mentally ill, promiscuous, lonely, insecure, and likely to be child
molesters, while lesbians have been described as aggressive and hostile toward
men. Positive characteristics are also part of the homosexual stereotype
including such traits as sensitivity, intelligence, honesty, imagination, and
neatness.
Recent research in social cognition has revealed the importance of stereotypes
as cognitive categories for imposing order and predictability on the world.
Some people feel the need for categorization so strongly that they increase
their liking for a person simply because she or he labels another as
homosexual. Homosexual persons who violate stereotypical expectations (e.g.,
masculine gay men and feminine lesbians) may actually be disliked. Such
nonconformity may not be noticed, however, since labeling itself can lead
people to perceive stereotypical behaviors, whether or not they occur. . . .
It frequently is assumed that feelings of personal threat result in strong
negative attitudes toward homosexuality, whereas lack of threat leads to
neutral or positive attitudes. This perspective often is associated with the
term homophobia, and it derives from a psychodynamic view that
prejudiced attitudes serve to reduce tension aroused by unconscious
conflicts.
Attitudes are likely to serve a defensive function when an individual perceives
some analogy between homosexual persons and her or his own unconscious
conflicts. Subsequently, that person responds to gay men and lesbians as a way
of externalizing inner conflicts and thereby reducing the anxiety associated
with them. The conflicts specific to antihomosexual prejudice presumably
involve a person's gender identity, sexual object choice, or both. For example,
unconscious conflicts about one's own sexuality or gender identity might be
attributed to lesbians and gay men through a process of projection. Such a
strategy permits people to externalize the conflicts and to reject their own
unacceptable urges by rejecting lesbians and gay men (who symbolize those
urges) without consciously recognizing the urges as their own. Since contact
with homosexual persons threatens to make conscious those thoughts that have
been repressed, it inevitably arouses anxiety in defensive individuals.
Consequently, defensive attitudes are likely to be negative.
Several psychodynamic explanations offered for attitudes toward lesbians and
gay men fit with the defensive function. Heterosexual men may envy gay men
because the latter are not constrained by the masculine ideal. Heterosexuals
may also envy the sexual freedom presumably enjoyed by lesbians and gay men. In
either case, the envy is presumably translated unconsciously into hostility. In
a similar vein, Cory (1951) also proposed that negative feelings toward
opposite-sex homosexuals result from heterosexuals' feelings of rejection as
potential sexual partners. Weinberg (1972) hypothesized that since many people
strive for vicarious immortality by having children, and since lesbians and gay
men are perceived (incorrectly) as having rejected this means for eluding the
finality of death, the latter evoke an unconscious fear of death.
Many particular empirical findings I have mentioned make sense if we assume
that negative attitudes often are based in part on a defensive function: the
finding that people are more negative toward homosexuals of their own sex than
toward those of the opposite sex (since same-sex homosexuals presumably are
more threatening); the positive correlations between hostile attitudes toward
homosexuality and variables such as authoritarianism' cognitive rigidity'
intolerance of ambiguity, and dogmatism (all of these personality traits
presumably indicate higher levels of defensiveness); and the positive
correlations between hostility and sex-guilt sexual conservatism, and
nonpermissiveness (all of which might indicate conflicts about sexuality). . .
.
McConahay and Hough (1976) defined symbolic racism as "the expression [by
whites], in terms of abstract ideological symbols and symbolic behaviors, [of]
the feeling that blacks are violating cherished values and making illegitimate
demands for changes in the racial status quo." This definition can be used to
delineate the third functional category of attitudes toward lesbians and gay
men. As with symbolic racism, symbolic sexual attitudes express the feeling
that cherished values are being violated and that illegitimate demands are
being made for changes in the status quo. I will expand the definition,
however, to include favorable attitudes that are based upon the belief that
discrimination and prejudice themselves violate the values of freedom and
equality.
Whether favorable or unfavorable, symbolic attitudes derive from socialization
experiences, past and present. They express values important to one's concept
of self, thereby helping individuals to establish their identity and affirm
their notion of the sort of person they perceive themselves to be, while
simultaneously mediating their relation to other important individuals and
reference groups. This is part of an ongoing social dialectic through which
one's sense of self develops while it also defines interpersonal
relationships.
The symbolic pattern is apparent in the empirical data already summarized.
Heterosexuals who express hostile attitudes toward homosexual persons also tend
to endorse traditional ideologies of family, sexuality' and sex roles, and
often are prejudiced against other minorities as well. That some of these same
findings also apply to the defensive function underscores the complex,
overdetermined nature of attitudes toward homosexual persons. Attitudes serving
different functions can be correlated with identical behaviors.
For persons with symbolic attitudes, certain reference groups appear to be
particularly influential. As already mentioned, people who are involved in
church groups (as indicated by frequent attendance at church services) reflect
the historical religious bias against lesbians and gay men, and this is
especially so for Christians. People who grew up in areas where higher
tolerance exists for diversity also hold more positive attitudes toward
lesbians and gay men; these include city-dwellers and people from the
northeastern and Pacific coastal regions of the United States The same studies
report that more tolerant attitudes are held by younger persons (whose cohorts'
values reflect the liberalism of the 1960s and 1970s) and by persons with more
education (who presumably have been exposed to liberal values on a college
campus). Finally, people are more tolerant of lesbians and gay men if their
parents also displayed tolerance.
| |
Excerpted with permission from "Beyond 'Homophobia': A Social Psychological Perspective on Attitudes Toward Lesbians and Gay Men" by Gregory M. Herek in the Journal of Homosexuality, Vol. 10, No. 1/2 (1984), pp. 1-15. Footnotes omitted.
| |
|