| | |
|
Richard Pillard is a professor of psychiatry at the Boston University School of
Medicine.
| |
A problem for those of us who favor a genetic basis for sexual orientation is
why, from an evolutionary point of view, gay attractions should exist at all.
My suggestion is that both orientations are genetically programmed, that both
appeared during the evolutionary history of our species and therefore may exist
at least in rudimentary form in our close primate relatives. This is one
reason that I read with interest descriptions of other primates' sexual
behavior. The study of same-sex behavior in animals is emerging from a void.
Almost every class of animals has individuals who copulate with members of
their own sex. But what does it mean? Does the mounting of one male monkey by
another male monkey express dominance, pair bonding, reassurance, sexual
attraction, or all of the above? Primatologist George Vasey makes an
interesting observation that homosexual mounting occurs rarely in New World
(plathrrhine) monkeys but is more frequent and more clearly expressed among Old
World (catarrhinc) primates, who are more recently evolved an d more closely
related to us. Catarrhine males will sometimes show a preferences for a male
partner and will compete with other males for such a partner.
Sheep are another species in which males occasionally show what looks like
preferential homosexual mate choice. Biologists Ann Perkins and James
Fitzgerald, working at the U.S. Sheep experiment Station ion Idaho, are
trying an interesting experiment. They identified rams who show an interest
in mounting other rams, electro-ejaculated them to collect sperm, and used the
sperm to impregnate ewes. The offspring rams, as they are interbred, will be
tested to see if they show a homosexual mounting choice more frequently which
they should if there are "gay genes" concentrated and passed on from parent and
grandparent ancestors.
This discussion so far still leaves bare the question why an orientation that
appears so inimical to reproduction might nevertheless persist in human or
animal populations. Homosexuality needs to demonstrate a survival benefit
sufficient to offset the reproductive cost that the orientation would be
expected to exact. Most reproductively disadvantageous traits occur because
of occasional and random mutations which are gradually selected out of the
kindred. Huntington's Diseas, for example, occurs in only about four people
per million. The gay/lesbian phenotype is far too common to be entirely the
result of deleterious mutation. if genetic, it must have undergone some degree
of favorable selections. One concept to understand about "reproductive
success" is that it can be realized by different strategies. The oak produces
thousands of acorns and is lucky if one or two grow to make acorns of their
own. The larger land mammals on the other hand, have relatively few offspring
and are metabolically expensive in guarding and protecting the relatively few
offspring that we are able to have.
This line of thinking, elaborated by biologists Robert Trivers and James
Weinrich, among others, is that gay people may have evolved character traits of
an altruistic nature that prompt them to work harder for the protection and
advancement of closely related family members rather than invest in having
children of their own. This idea, called "kin altruism," results from the
simple calculus that two nephews or nieces are genetically equivalent to one
son or daughter. Reproductive sacrifice explainable by a theory of kin
altruism has been demonstrated in some animal species but remains speculative
for humans, most of whom, sad to say, seem rather short in the department of
altruism.. (Cf., Jim Weinrich, Sexual Landscapes: Why We Are What We Are,
Why We Love Whom We Love, 1987.)
Ray Blanchard and his colleagues at the Clarke Institute in Toronto recently
analyzed thousands of families and found that gay men have a later birth order
than straight. Specifically, the gay men had more older brothers but not more
older sisters once the older brothers were taken into account. A
psycho-developmental explanation springs to mind: A young boy might develop
homosexual attractions more readily if an older brother is in the picture. But
a biological explanation is also on the horizon. The placenta supporting every
human pregnancy has small protein fingers that engage the mother's bloodstream
by digging into the wall of her uterus. When the placenta is shed, some of the
placental cells remain in the uterine wall and can be demonstrated to be alive
and well years later. These cells came form the fetus and contain its genome.
It is possible that some cells of an early pregnancy remain behind and, by an
unexplained mechanism, try to capture the resources of subsequent pregnancies,
a sort of biological primogeniture? I like this odd (and admittedly remote)
possibility because it shows how much is still unexpected in the biology of
human sexuality.
Behavior genetics applied to sexual orientation encounters a variety of
criticisms. In this debate, I feel like a detective who's presenting his case
to the district attorney. I have scraps and clues, hypothesis and sometimes
hang together but that also have gaps. But the DA must be a skeptic. He has
to put my case before a jury whom he must convince beyond a reasonable doubt.
The standard of proof rises as the investigation proceeds. (Thanks to Jimm
Weinrich for the analogy.) The most I can say is that no one has yet put
forward evidence that is devastating to my case.
s
There is one criticism I want at least to touch on, namely that genetics
theories pander to homophobia. If "gay genes" are discovered, how much easier
will it be to eradicate them? Opinion surveys are unanimous t hat people who
endorse a genetic hypothesis are more sympathetic to lesbians and gays than
people who hole to a purely environmental theory. Still, technology ahs a way
of biting back. No scientific knowledge is risk-free, and this must surely
include genetic investigations of sexual orientation. One might take a sort of
verse comfort in knowing that homophobia, like racism (an all the
xenophobias), exists regardless of whatever might be considered "the facts"
of the moment. Research on human sexuality will, by its nature, evoke
resistance and fear, to some extent legitimately. Here is where I think
social science research has a valuable role. There ought to be funded
research programs not only to unravel the genome but to unravel the ethical and
psychological dilemmas that accompany the new insights that behavior genetics
will generate.
Scientific research is permeated by values. Mine are that it is better to know
something than not to know it. I believe that same-sex desire will remain a
topic of interest and study for a long time. I'm confident that biological
knowledge will prove it to be a valuable trait, selected by evolution precisely
because it contributes some quality that was useful, perhaps even essential, to
the sudden ascendancy of human beings among all the primates.
| |
Excerpted with permission from "The Genetic Theory of Sexual Orientation" in
the Harvard Gay and Lesbian Review, Winter 1997, pp. 61-67.
| |
|